Christopher T Smith.com
  • Home
  • About Me
  • Leadership
  • Reflections
  • Career Development Research
  • Neuroscience Research
  • Published Research
  • Press
  • Presentations
  • Job Search Resources
  • Funding Resources
  • Subscribe to Newsletter
  • Contact

Reflections Blog

Stress & the Brain: How genetics affects whether you are more likely to wilt under pressure

6/18/2020

1 Comment

 
​Neuroscience
​
Adapted from a post appearing as part of my new
Brain & Behavior blog on LifeApps.
Don’t stress. Stop stressing out. Relieve your stress.
Is stress always a bad thing?
Is there any truth to the fact that some people thrive under stress? Actually, yes.
Stress & the Brain
First, when we are under stress, a variety of biological processes are taking place. Here, I am going to focus on a particular role stress can play in the brain. Under psychological stress the amygdala activates stress pathways in the brain and the hypothalamus and brainstem release high amounts of norepinephrine and dopamine. While these chemicals have a range of effects on the brain, I want to focus this discussion on how they impact the prefrontal cortex (PFC). 

The PFC is the most frontal part of the brain, sitting above our eyes. This part of the brain has seen rapid evolutionary development and is much larger in primates, including humans, than other mammals. The PFC plays a major role in planning, higher-order thinking, problem solving, and simulating/anticipating the results of actions we may or may not undertake. 
Picture
The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is the most recently evolved portion of the brain, sitting above the eyes.
The PFC is also important in cognitive control, which can be thought of as “staying on task” despite the presence of potential distractors. When you need to concentrate really hard on a complex math problem on a test, your PFC is highly engaged. A key function this brain region must undertake, then, is maintaining relevant, important information over a long enough time period for you to act on it...think holding on to the intermediate product of a mathematical formula while the next piece of information is being added (12 times 2 is 24, plus 6 is 30, and divide by three, equals 10). 

The neurotransmitters dopamine and norepinephrine play key roles in maintaining this critical information (a concept known as working memory) in the PFC. 
Picture
Molecular structure of the neurotransmitter dopamine.
Picture
Molecular structure of the neurotransmitter norepinephrine.
Dopamine, Norepinephrine, & the Prefrontal Cortex
To summarize an incredible amount of work by the likes of Amy Arnsten at Yale University and others (see also), dopamine is thought to be important in repressing the “noise” (distractions) in PFC brain circuits while norepinephrine is believed to enhance the “signal”. 

The effects of these two neurotransmitters on the PFC and ultimately behavior are nonlinear. In fact, there is much evidence that the effects of these chemicals on the brain follow an inverted-U relationship where too little or too much dopamine or norepinephrine in the PFC leads to sub/supra-optimal functioning of this area of the brain. Thus, an intermediate level of these chemicals is best for optimal PFC function. 
Stress increases the levels of dopamine and norepinephrine in the brain.
The inverted-U model for dopamine and norepinephrine effects on the PFC is important given the fact that stress increases the level of both of these chemicals in this area of the brain.
​
So, the optimal balance of dopamine and norepinephrine may be thrown off under stress. 


But, this isn’t the complete story.
Biological Differences in PFC Dopamine Signalling 
What we also know from a great deal of research is that there are biological differences in neurotransmitter signaling in the brain. More is known about how dopamine signaling varies across individuals based on their genetic variation (and see; and also), which I will focus on for the remainder of this piece.
Picture
A single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the COMT gene results in a change in the amino acid structure (valine to methionine, Val to Met) of the COMT enzyme. This change results in a COMT enzyme with lower activity (act) that then breaks down dopamine (DA) and other catecholamines less efficiently. The result is higher levels of DA in the PFC.
Genetic Variation in COMT Affects PFC Dopamine Levels 
I will focus specifically on an interesting genetic variant in the COMT gene. What is COMT? It is an enzyme that helps to break down dopamine, as well as other chemicals with similar molecular structures, and regulate its level in the body and brain. A single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the COMT gene (G->A DNA substitution resulting in a Val->Met amino acid variation in the protein; and denoted as the Val158Met COMT variant) results in a differential stability of the COMT enzyme, allowing it to more or less efficiently breakdown dopamine. And while COMT breaks down a variety of catecholamines, including norepinephrine, epinephrine, and dopamine, it plays a majority role in regulating dopamine levels in the PFC. Those with the COMT Val polymorphism have higher enzymatic activity and therefore lower levels of tonic PFC dopamine. In contrast, the COMT Met polymorphism results in lower COMT activity and therefore higher levels of tonic PFC dopamine. 
Picture
Schematic illustrating how variation in COMT ultimately affects dopamine (DA) signaling in the PFC.
Interestingly, this particular COMT SNP has recently evolved in humans and both polymorphisms (Val & Met) are quite common in the human population. This suggests both versions of the COMT SNP had a useful purpose, evolutionarily. One popular hypothesis for the common occurrence of these COMT variants is the Worrier vs Warrior explanation. 

Several studies (see comprehensive review here) have demonstrated that individuals with the Met version of the COMT enzyme perform well in cognitively demanding tasks but have an enhanced vulnerability to stress (worrier); they wilt under pressure. 
In contrast, individuals with the Val version of COMT have better stress resiliency (warrior). 
Picture
Picture
Stress elevates the level of catecholamines like dopamine (DA) in the PFC, resulting in a shift in where individuals with the ValVal or MetMet COMT polymorphism find themselves on the inverted-U function between PFC DA and task performance, where intermediate DA associates with optimal performance. While an individual with the MetMet polymorphism may (under certain conditions) be in the optimal range of PFC DA and task performance under normal conditions, stress pushes their DA levels into supra-optimal levels, degrading task performance. In contrast, stress-induced increases in DA may help shift those with the ValVal polymorphism closer to optimal DA levels and thus improve task performance. 
This is thought to be due to the fact Val individuals have lower tonic dopamine and that the dopamine boost that occurs under stress moves these individuals toward a more optimal level of dopamine for performing cognitively demanding tasks. To further support these points, a large study in Taiwan has shown ValVal (remember, humans have two copies of each gene, one from Mom & one from Dad) individuals perform better on a stressful, standardized test administered to 10th graders across the country each year. 

And it’s not just an effect on dopamine the Val158Met COMT polymorphism provides. Research has shown that ValVal individuals show lower physiological stress reactivity than MetMet individuals. 

Taken together, these data suggest that individuals with two copies of the Met allele will generally perform poorer under stressful conditions than those with two copies of the Val allele while those with a copy of each allele will fall somewhere in between.
Is your genotype destiny? 
Should those individuals with the Met polymorphism in their COMT gene resign themselves to doing poorly on big standardized tests; wilting under pressure? 

​The short answer? No. The long answer? We are more than our genetics.

First, our genetics interact with other aspects of our biology to ultimately produce behavior. My own research has shown that COMT genotype interacts with age to affect Now vs Later decision making. We interpreted this in context of the inverted-U relationship between dopamine and PFC function as dopamine levels are known to decline with age. So, a particular genetic setup that leads to supra-optimal dopamine levels when one is young may result in more optimal levels as one ages and dopamine “falls” down the curve toward more optimal levels. 
Picture
Dopamine levels naturally decline with age. Thus, where one's COMT genetics positions them in terms of optimal PFC function will shift over the lifespan with ValVal individuals in the above example falling out of the optimal dopamine range while those with the MetMet polymorphism may fall down into a more optimal intermediate level of PFC dopamine.
Note, that what level of PFC dopamine is "optimal" for various cognitive tasks will differ based on a variety of environmental factors. Thus, the COMT ValVal polymorphism may be more optimal in some situations and MetMet more optimal in others. 

Biology is just one part of the equation. 
​The Importance of Mindset
Mindset, or how an individual reacts to the biological changes that accompany stress, is also critical.    

It has been shown that taking a stress-is-enhancing mindset leads to better affective and cognitive outcomes than a stress-is-debilitating mindset.  
COMT genetic variation has been shown to mediate the effect of a stress-is-enhancing mindset manipulation on affect and cognition such that those with two copies of the Met allele were more responsive to the manipulation than those with two copies of the Val allele. MetMet individuals can more easily develop a stress-is-enhancing mindset.

And while your COMT genetics may affect how well mindset manipulations work, anyone can take steps to re-frame their stressful experiences in such a way as to see stress as more of a benefit than a detriment. For example, treat your stress as something you learn from rather than dwelling on the negative aspects.
Final Thoughts
In closing, genetic variation in dopamine signaling plays a role in how we perform under cognitively demanding tasks. Evolutionarily speaking, it made sense for some people to perform well under pressure (Val warriors) while others performed better under baseline, unstressed conditions (Met worriers). We should embrace the genetic diversity inherent in this and other behaviors but also realize biology is only one determinant of behavior. Our mindset and how we frame the effect of stress on us is also critical and, in fact, has biological effects on our stress response. 

The data presented here reflect a theme common in the brain and human behavior: behavior is modulated by both our biology and environment. Behavior is complicated and so to understand it, we need to look beyond merely our genes, proteins, and cells. Especially when it comes to human behavior, our environment and experiences affect our biology and behavior.

​None of these relationships are simple, which is what makes studying them so interesting.  
Further Reading:
Catechol-O-Methyltransferase moderates effect of stress mindset on affect and cognition

Changing Stress Mindset Through Stressjam: A Virtual Reality Game Using Biofeedback

Quantitative role of COMT in dopamine clearance in the prefrontal cortex of freely moving mice

The influence of Val158Met COMT on physiological stress responsivity

COMT genetic variation affects fear processing: psychophysiological evidence

The efficacy of stress reappraisal interventions on stress responsivity: A meta-analysis and systematic review of existing evidence

Rethinking stress: the role of mindsets in determining the stress response
​

The catechol-O-methyltransferase Val(158)Met polymorphism modulates fronto-cortical dopamine turnover in early Parkinson's disease: a PET study
​

Site-Specific Role of Catechol-O-Methyltransferase in Dopamine Overflow within Prefrontal Cortex and Dorsal Striatum
​

Older age may offset genetic influence on affect: The COMT polymorphism and affective well-being across the life span
1 Comment

    Author

    A neuroscientist by training, I now work to improve the career readiness of graduate students and postdoctoral scholars.

      Subscribe to Reflections Newsletter

    Subscribe to Newsletter

    Archives

    October 2024
    April 2024
    March 2024
    February 2024
    January 2024
    December 2023
    November 2023
    October 2023
    September 2023
    August 2023
    May 2023
    March 2023
    February 2023
    January 2023
    December 2022
    November 2022
    October 2022
    September 2022
    August 2022
    June 2022
    May 2022
    April 2022
    March 2022
    January 2022
    December 2021
    October 2021
    September 2021
    August 2021
    July 2021
    May 2021
    April 2021
    March 2021
    February 2021
    January 2021
    December 2020
    November 2020
    October 2020
    September 2020
    August 2020
    July 2020
    June 2020
    May 2020
    March 2020
    February 2020
    January 2020
    December 2019
    November 2019
    September 2019
    August 2019
    July 2019
    May 2019
    April 2019

    Categories

    All
    Academic Job Search
    Artificial Intelligence
    Career Development
    Career Exploration
    Creativity
    Data Science
    Future Of Work
    Innovation
    International Concerns
    Job Search
    Life Advice
    Neuroscience
    NIH BEST Blog Rewind
    Opinion
    Personalized Medicine
    PhD Career Pathways
    Professional Development
    Scientific Workforce
    Sports
    Tools & Resources
    Welcome

    RSS Feed

Science

Career Development Research
​
Neuroscience Research


Publications

Writing

​Reflections Blog

Other Posts

Press, Resources, & Contact

Press                                                       Contact

Job Search Resources         Funding Resources

Subscribe to Reflections Newsletter 
© COPYRIGHT 2025.
​ALL RIGHTS RESERVED.